Shiken: JALT Testing & Evaluation SIG Newsletter
Vol. 12 No. 1. Jan. 2008. (p. 46-54) [ISSN 1881-5537]
PDF PDF Version

Suggested Answers for Assessment Literacy Self-Study Quiz #5
by Tim Newfields

Here are some suggested answers for the questions about testing, statistics, and assessment from the April 2008 issue of SHIKEN.
If you feel an answer is unclear or conclusion is incorrect, please contact the editor.

Part I: Open Questions

1 Q: A person with a hearing disability is asked to take an EFL placement test . . . . what's the most ethical way to rate such an individual?

   A: First, we need to ascertain the test purpose and context. If the sole purpose is classroom streaming and the teachers administering the test already are familiar with the level of each class, then an informal placement interview might work well in lieu of the standard test. Basing the placement choice entirely on the reading test scores wouldn't be a wise option because EFL reading skills do not necessarily correlate highly with listening skills (Basabas-Ikeguchi,1988).
* If we are talking about a high-stakes test for which a comprehensive score is required, several options exist. If an examinee's hearing ability is only partly impaired, an assistive listening device (ALD) could be employed. If the hearing loss is total, the best option might be to use some type of captioning system. The STEP-Eiken provides captioning services for applicants unable to hear, but unfortunately ETS doesn't (ETS, 2007). Since an estimated .48% of the population is partly deaf and a further .18% is completely so (Holt, Hotto, and Cole, 1994), it is worth thinking about accommodations for those with auditory impairments in advance.

Further reading:

Basabas-Ikeguchi, C. (1988). Analysis of reading and listening comprehension skills in different language environments. Unpublished master's thesis, Dokkyo University. ERIC #: ED355807.

Burns, E. (1998). Test accommodations for students with disabilities. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

ETS. (2007) 2007-2008 Bulletin supplement for test takers with disability. Retrieved April 12, 2008 from www.ets.org/disability/

Holt, J., Hotto, S., Cole, K. (1994). Demographic aspects of hearing impairment: Questions and answers. (Third Edition). Retrieved April 12, 2008 from http://gri.gallaudet.edu/Demographics/factsheet.html#Q1/

[ p. 46 ]

2 Q: How should an oral proficiency interviewee with a likely stuttering disorder be rated?

   A: Since it is important to ascertain whether the stuttering is a pervasive speech impediment or simply a manifestation of nervousness due to undertaking a foreign language exam, it would be ideal to speak with the examinee in his/her native tongue briefly. If the stuttering is a persistent, global phenomena then it is likely that a handicap is present for which accommodations are due. If the person speaks their native language without undue stammering, then the speech impairment may be a result of social anxiety rather than a defined impairment. In that case, no special accommodations would be needed and the examinee's fluency rating would subsequently drop.
* The issue, however, is actually more complex since some forms of stuttering are episodic and oral interviewers are not qualified to provide clinical diagnoses. For such reasons self-diagnosis should be the main criteria for deciding whether or not a handicap exists among adults. If a person indicates that they have a stuttering disorder, then they are legally entitled to "reasonable accommodation" or "adaptive measures" from the agency in question (ELSA, 2000). When completing a test application, space should be provided for examinees to indicate whether they have any disabilities requiring special accommodation.
* What specific accommodations should be made when rating the oral fluency of those who chronically stammer? Here the issue becomes complex because disorders vary widely. One option would be to listen to the output as if no disorder existed – to essentially ignore the features of the output that could be ascribed to stuttering and try to rate the remaining speech features. This is not an easy process and it seems that oral proficiency raters vary widely in their responses to stammering.

Further reading:

ELSA. (2000). ELSA Links – Discrimination. Retrieved April 13, 2008 from http://www.stuttering.ws/links/discrim_eu.htm

Tyrer, A. (2007, September 23). Oral assessments, and assessed presentations. Retrieved April 13, 2008 from http://www.stammeringlaw.org.uk/education/oral.htm


3 Q: One instructor of a basic "English communication" class awards credit if his students indicate that they've recently donated blood. Any content validity issues here?

   A: Teachers often use grades as levers to induce desired behaviors. If the grading process is ethical and in line with the curricular goals and the criteria for performance are communicated clearly to all stakeholders, there is no problem.

[ p. 47 ]


* In the scenario presented here, however, several concerns arise. First, the curricular goals are not expressed clearly – the syllabus is far too vague about expected outcomes. Moreover, the relevance of donating blood to those curricular goals is not established. How does donating blood pertain to English proficiency? Finally, this grading system presumes all students are healthy and able to donate blood, which might not be the case. The teacher is penalizing those whose health condition (or religious beliefs) does not enable them to make blood donations. This case illustrates how teachers need to be cautious about offering incentive points to induce students to undertake specific behaviors: it is all too easy to dish out points for actions not directly related to curricular goals.

Further reading:

Anderson, L. W. (2002, November) Curricular alignment: A re-examination. Theory Into Practice, 41, (4) 255 - 260. ERIC Document # EJ667162.

Barrie, S., Brew, A., McCulloch, M. (1999). Qualitatively different conceptions of criteria used to assess student learning. Paper presented at the 1999 Australian Association for Research in Education. Retrieved April 14, 2008 from http://www.aare.edu.au/99pap/bre99209.htm


4 Q: What further information should be provided to end users of ExpertRating's English Speaking Test, which is online at www.expertrating.com/english-speaking-test.asp?

   A: Let's start by considering the construct that's reputedly being measured. The test claims to measure "correct pronunciation in [American] English". However, this claim implies that there is only one "correct" American English pronunciation. According to the University of Arizona Language Samples Project (2001) and Kun (2007) that is simply not the case. There are many regional and ethnic varieties of American English and no single dialect can be regarded as "correct". Hence this exam appears to have a design flaw from the onset.
* Second, this test gives no information about how pronunciation ability is measured. Are there trained human raters or is the rating entirely based on a computer speech recognition system? If human raters are used, how many raters are employed and what are their evaluation criteria? The rating criteria for this exam are far too opaque.
* Another egrerious point about this test is that it completely neglects to mention what validation criteria, if any, are employed for this test. No descriptive statistics about its reliability or validity are offered to end users. Examinees have a right to know how well the scores on a given exam correlate with other widely used measures of English proficiency.
* In short, this examination has a long way to go before it can be considered a valid, professional, or ethical measure of the ability to speak English. Commercial test developers should devote at least as much energy to test validation as they do to marketing.

[ p. 48 ]

Further reading:

Garcia, P. A. (1987). The competency testing mine field: Validation, legal and ethical issues with implications for minorities. ERIC Document # ED336967

Kun, T. (2007). American regional accent map. Retrieved April 15, 2008 from http://freeshells.ch/~xavier/accentmap/

Saar, H. (2005, January 17). Validation guidelines for test developers. Retrieved April 15, 2008 from www.qalspell.ttu.ee/Validation%20Guidelines%20for%20Test%20Developers.doc

University of Arizona Language Samples Project. (2001). Varieties of English. Retrieved April 15, 2008 from http://www.ic.arizona.edu/~lsp/main.html

Part II: Multiple Choice Questions


1 Q: Which of the following is not a feature traditional conversation analysis?


   A: Data quantification (Option D) is not a feature of traditional conversation analysis; the focus of conversation analyses is generally on the descriptive features of specific interactions rather than their frequency. Weider and Lawrence (1993) argue against attempting to quantify conversations because of the idiolectic nature of human communication and the small sample sizes generally involved in CA studies. Despite this, CA studies make frequent use of pseudo-quantifying terms such as 'regularly', 'often', 'commonly', 'rarely' etc. (Ten Have, 2000). A few researchers such as West (1984) go further and actually quantify their data to the extent of mentioning percentiles when describing male/female discourse patterns. The question of whether (and of course how) to quantify conversational data is an ongoing controversy. Citing works by Stivers (2001, 2002), TESOL Quarterly advises writers wishing to use quantification to "ensure that it only follows careful analysis of the individual cases that are being quantified, with categories for quantification emerging from this analysis of individual cases" rather than any a priori decision.

Further reading:

Stivers, T. (2001). Negotiating who presents the problem: Next speaker selection in pediatric encounters. Journal of Communication, 51, 252-282.

Stivers, T. (2002). Presenting the problem in pediatric encounters: "Symptoms only" versus "candidate diagnosis" presentations. Health Communication, 14, 299-338.

[ p. 49 ]

Ten Have, P. (2000, July 3). Methodological issues in conversation analysis. Retrieved April 16, 2008 from http://www2.fmg.uva.nl/emca/mica.htm

TESOL Quarterly. (n.d.). Qualitative research: Conversation analysis guidelines. Retrieved from April 16, 2008 from http://www.tesol.org/s_tesol/sec_document.asp?CID=476&DID=2154

West, C. (1984) Routine complications: Trouble with talk between doctors and patients. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Wieder, D. L. (1993). On the compound questions raised by attempts to quantify conversation analysis' phenomena, part 2: The issue of incommensurability. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 26 (2) 213-26. ERIC #: EJ464150.


2 Q: If a person takes a multiple reading test and selects an answer simply because the other choices do not seem correct, it is a _____________ strategy.


   A: According to Cohen (2007, p. 93) the case above would be Option (B) – an example of a test-management strategy. Other examples of such strategies include using a clock during an exam, re-reading a text passage, or guessing answers on the basis of background knowledge. Test-management strategies represent attempts to maximize an individual's resources to score well on a test. Cohen and Upton (2006) specify 28 different test-management strategies among TOEFL® examinees.
*Testwiseness is said to occur when examinees rely on secondary cues from test passages to perform above their actual abilities (Millman, Bishop, & Ebel 1965, cited by Edwards, 2003). First proposed by Thorndike in 1951, sample test-wiseness strategies include avoiding answers with words such as "all" or "none" or selecting test items which have more detail without knowing whether the answer is actually correct. Ideally, well-designed tests should not be susceptible to test-wiseness strategies. In actually, most examinations do contain at least some faulty items unduly favoring test-wise examinees (Rogers and Bateson, 1991; Mahamed, Gregory, Austin, and Dan, 2006).
* Language learner strategies are not related to testing per se; they represent broader attempts to gain linguistic and sociolinguistic competence in a target language (Tarone, 1983, cited by Lessard-Clouston, 1997). Sample language learning strategies might include asking questions when information isn't understood or modifying L2 output to better accommodate prevailing social-cultural norms.
* It might be worth mentioning that bifurcation between test-management strategies and testwiseness strategies is not entirely satisfactory and not all authors favor this distinction (Gu, 1996, cited by Bremner, 1997).

Further reading:

[ p. 50 ]

Bremner, S. (1997, Autumn). Language learning strategies and language proficiency: causes or outcomes? Perspectives (9). Retrieved from April 18, 2008 from http://sunzi1.lib.hku.hk/hkjo/view/10/1000125.pdf

Cohen, A. D. & Upton, T. A. (2006). Strategies in responding to the new TOEFL reading tasks. TOEFL Monograph No. MS-33. Princeton, NJ: ETS. Retrieved from April 17, 2008 from http://www.ets.org/Media/Research/pdf/RR-06-06.pdf

Cohen, A. D. (2007) The coming of age for research on test-taking strategies. In J. Fox, et al (Eds.) Language Testing Reconsidered. Ottawa, Ontario: University of Ottawa Press., pp. 89 - 112.

Edwards, B. (2003, August). An examination of factors contributing to a reduction in race-based subgroup differences on a constructed response paper-and-pencil test of achievement. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis at Texas A&M University. Retrieved from April 17, 2008 from http://txspace.tamu.edu/bitstream/handle/1969.1/128/etd-tamu-2003B-2003062513-Edwa-1.pdf?sequence=1

Gu, P.Y. (1996). Robin Hood in SLA: What has the learning strategy researcher taught us? Asian Journal of English Language Teaching, 6, 1-29.

Lessard-Clouston, M. (1997, December) Language Learning Strategies: An Overview for L2 Teachers. The Internet TESL Journal, 3 (12). Retrieved from April 17, 2008 from http://iteslj.org/Articles/Lessard-Clouston-Strategy.html

Mahamed, A., Gregory, P., Austin, Z., & Dan, L. (2006, December). Testwiseness among international pharmacy graduates and Canadian senior pharmacy students. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 70 (6), p. 131. Retrieved from April 17, 2008 from http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1803693

Millman, J., Bishop, C. H., & Ebel, R. (1965). An analysis of test wiseness. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 25, 707-726.

Rogers, W. T.; Bateson, D. J. (1991, April). The Influence of Test-Wiseness on Performance of High School Seniors on School Leaving Examinations. Applied Measurement in Education, 4, 159 - 183.

Tarone, E. (1983). Some thoughts on the notion of 'communication strategy'. In C. Faerch & G. Kasper (Eds.), Strategies in Interlanguage Communication (pp. 61-74). London: Longman.



3 Q: Which of the following statements is/are true about probability values (often called p-values)?

[ p. 51 ]


   A: Although Option (A) could be considered a viable answer, in some ways Option (D) is also valid because no single statistic by itself can provide us with enough information to meaningfully describe an entire set of data. Traditional p-values should only be used along with a wide range of other statistics to discern the likelihood of a result being due to random noise or some significantly different. Increasingly, some people question whether or not p-values should be used at all: better means to detect the probability of random chance exist.
* According to Dixon (2000), p-values are often misused and in fact they might not be the best tool for describing whether research results arise from random chance. He argues that likelihood ratios (often expressed with the Greek letter λ – lower case lambda), expressed in the formula below, offer a better way to gauge significant research results.
Dixon's formula

This formula does not appear to be widely used today, but another alternative proposed by Killeen (2005) is more widely accepted. Note how this formula below differs from the previous formula:
Killeen's formula

* According to Killeen (2005), prep values avoid problematic parametric inferences inherent in traditional p-values and provide a viable way to detect random noise. The procedure for calculating prep through SPSS is described in detail by Wright (2008).
* P-values – which are indeed flawed historical relics – do not indicate the direction or the strength of a correlation, nor provide any clues about causality. Under the best of conditions, they offer an inkling of the likelihood of some result being due to random chance provided that a test is well-designed and the sampling is also done well. However, the best of conditions is seldom met and most tests we are likely to encounter have some types of design flaws. For such reasons, considerable caution needs to be used when interpreting p-values.

[ p. 52 ]

JD Brown (2008, p. 36-41) offered two examples of how p-value results could be misleading in this issue of SHIKEN. If you look through the literature closely, it is not hard to find plenty of others that exist.

Further reading:

Brown, J.D. (2008, April). Statistics Corner. Questions and answers about language testing statistics: Effect size and eta squared. Shiken: JALT Testing & Evaluation SIG Newsletter, 12 (2) 36 - 41. Retrieved from April 18, 2008 from http://jalt.org/test/bro_28.htm

Dixon, P. (2003, September). The p-value fallacy and how to avoid it. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 189-202. Retrieved from April 18, 2008 from http://www.psych.ualberta.ca/~pdixon/Home/Preprints/pValue.pdf

Dixon, P. (2000, July). The p-value fallacy: Why inferential statistics don't describe results. Paper presented at the joint meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society of Great Britain and the Canadian Society for Brain, Behaviour, and Cognitive Science, Cambridge, UK. Retrieved from April 18, 2008 from http://www.psych.ualberta.ca/~pdixon/Home/Presentations/pValues/pValues.htm

Killeen, P. R. (2005, May). An Alternative to Null-Hypothesis Significance Tests. Psychological Science, 16 (5) 345-353. Retrieved from April 18, 2008 from http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1473027

Wright, D. (2008, March 3). Killeen's prep. Retrieved from April 18, 2008 from http://www.sussex.ac.uk/Users/danw/masters/statistical%20analysis/killeen.htm [inactive link]


4 Q: Which of the following usually does not lead to score inflation?

   A: Since random marking errors – Option (C) – work both ways, they should not lead to score inflation. The likelihood of someone benefiting from a random marking error is as great as the chance that they might be hurt by it. All of the other factors mentioned above can lead to test score inflation. So can poorly designed test questions which are vulnerable to testwiseness.

5 Q: Which of the following statements is true about power in a statistical sense?

[ p. 53 ]

   A: According to Trochim (2006) and Jacobs (2006) effect size – Option (C) – does have an impact on statistical power.
* Effect size does not refer to a single measure, but rather a host of indices to gauge the strength of the relationship between two variables. Common measures of effect size include Pearson's R, Cohen's d, Cramer's V, and Hedge's g. Some researchers such as Mousavi (p. 413), however, define effect size more narrowly as the mean score for an experimental group minus the mean score for a control group divided by the standard deviation for the control group. That could be likened to a Z-score ranging from 0 to 1. The more statistically powerful a test is, the less prone it is to a Type II error – falsely rejecting a null hypothesis.

Further reading:

Becker, L. (2000, March 21). Effect size. Retrieved from April 19, 2008 from http://web.uccs.edu/lbecker/Psy590/es.htm

Jacobs, R. (2006, December 19). The concepts of statistical power and effect size. Retrieved on April 19, 2008 from http://www83.homepage.villanova.edu/richard.jacobs/EDU%208603/lessons/stastical%20power.html

Meta Analysis. (2002). In S. A. Mousavi An Encyclopedic Dictionary of Language Testing. (3rd Ed.). (pp. 411-413). Taipei: Tung Hua Book Company.

Trochim, W. M.K. (2006). Research methods knowledge base: Statistical power. Retrieved on April 19, 2008 from http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/power.php


NEWSLETTER: Topic IndexAuthor IndexTitle IndexDate Index
TEVAL SIG: Main Page Background Links Network Join
last Main Page next
HTML: http://jalt.org/test/SSA5.htm   /   PDF: http://jalt.org/test/PDF/SSA5.pdf
ASSESSMENT LITERACY ONLINE SELF-STUDY QUIZES:

Quiz 1: Qs   / As    *    2: Qs   / As    *    3: Qs   As    *    4: Qs   As    *    5: Qs   / As    6: Qs   / As   *    7: Qs   / As    8: Qs   / As   9: Qs   / As   10: Qs   / As  

[ p. 54 ]